Psychology Dictionary
  • Dictionary
    • A
    • B
    • C
    • D
    • E
    • F
    • I
    • J
    • K
    • L
    • M
    • N
    • O
    • P
    • R
    • S
    • T
    • U
    • W
    • Y
    • Z
  • Health Topics
    • ADHD
    • Anxiety Disorders
    • Bipolar Disorder
    • Breast Cancer
    • Depression
    • Diabetes
    • Dissociative Disorders
    • Epilepsy
    • Insomnia
    • Neurology
    • Oncology
    • PCP
    • Pediatrics
    • Personality Disorders
    • Primary Care
    • Schizophrenia
    • Sleepdisorders
    • Substance Abuse Disorders
Aa
Psychology Dictionary
Aa
Search
  • Dictionary
    • A
    • B
    • C
    • D
    • E
    • F
    • I
    • J
    • K
    • L
    • M
    • N
    • O
    • P
    • R
    • S
    • T
    • U
    • W
    • Y
    • Z
  • Health Topics
    • ADHD
    • Anxiety Disorders
    • Bipolar Disorder
    • Breast Cancer
    • Depression
    • Diabetes
    • Dissociative Disorders
    • Epilepsy
    • Insomnia
    • Neurology
    • Oncology
    • PCP
    • Pediatrics
    • Personality Disorders
    • Primary Care
    • Schizophrenia
    • Sleepdisorders
    • Substance Abuse Disorders
Follow US
© PscyhologyDictionary.org. All Rights Reserved.
Home > P > PROTOTYPE in Psychology
P

PROTOTYPE in Psychology

By N., Sam M.S.

It's a concept to comprehend how people classify and identify things, concepts, or experiences based on mental models called prototypes. The greatest or most typical samples of a category that people use as a point of reference are known as prototypes. (Rosch, 1973).

Contents
Why is it important?Positive and negative effectsHow are they formed?Prototype approach to categorizationReal-life applicationsLimitationsFAQs

Also in concept formation, the best example of a category.

PROTOTYPE in Psychology: "The mountain gorilla is a prototype of all gorillas."

Why is it important?

We may digest information and make judgments more quickly and effectively by using prototypes as cognitive shortcuts. They are crucial because by comparing new experiences to our preexisting mental representations, they help us navigate challenging environments and make sense of them. (Medin & Schaffer, 1978).

Positive and negative effects

Positive aspects of the prototype effect include the ability to quickly process information and make decisions based on generalizations. It allows us to navigate new situations and recognize patterns without having to analyze every detail.

The prototype effect, however, can also have negative impacts like stereotyping, prejudice, and confirmation bias. When we rely too much on prototypes, we may ignore significant distinctions between people or circumstances, which could result in bad decisions and negative outcomes. (Hamilton & Sherman, 1994).

How are they formed?

Cognitive frameworks called prototype theories explain how people create mental models, or prototypes, and classify things, events, or concepts based on those models. Prototype theories come in various forms, each of which explains how prototypes are created and used in various contexts.

The concept known as the "exemplar theory" asserts that categorization is based on comparisons with particular examples, or "exemplars," of a category that we have previously experienced. As opposed to using a generalized bird prototype, we might, for instance, compare a new bird to specific birds we have already observed (such as robins or sparrows) when categorizing it.

Family Resemblance Theory: Rosch and Mervis (1975) put out this theory, which emphasizes the significance of common characteristics among category members. This idea states that even when no single attribute is shared by all members of a category, prototypes are still created based on the degree of similarity among category members. For instance, the term "furniture" encompasses items like chairs, tables, and sofas that have a variety of features in common but perhaps no single distinguishing feature.

Prototype approach to categorization

The prototype approach to categorization is a cognitive process that involves comparing new information or experiences with existing prototypes in order to assign them to appropriate categories. This approach has several key aspects:

Graded Membership: In prototype theory, categories are not rigidly defined but have graded membership, meaning that some members are considered more central or typical than others (Rosch, 1975). For example, a robin is a more prototypical bird than an ostrich.

Context Dependence: Categorization based on prototypes is sensitive to context, which can influence the perceived typicality of category members (Barsalou, 1987). For instance, a penguin may be seen as a more prototypical bird in an Antarctic context compared to a tropical one.

Category Flexibility: Prototype theory acknowledges that categories can be flexible and change over time as new information is acquired or prototypes are revised (Medin & Ortony, 1989). For example, a person's prototype of a smartphone may evolve as they are exposed to new models and features.

Real-life applications

Areas where prototype psychology plays a huge role"

Marketing and Advertising: Advertisers often use prototypes to create effective marketing campaigns by appealing to the most representative or idealized versions of products or consumer experiences (Keller, 1993).

Education and Learning: Understanding prototypes can help educators design curricula and teaching strategies that facilitate learning by building on students' existing mental representations and promoting effective categorization (Posner & Keele, 1968).

Artificial Intelligence and Machine Learning: Prototype theory has influenced the development of AI systems and machine learning algorithms that rely on categorization and pattern recognition, such as nearest-neighbor algorithms and clustering techniques (Aha, Kibler, & Albert, 1991).

Social Psychology: Prototype psychology has been applied to the study of social cognition, including the formation and impact of stereotypes, prejudice, and social categorization (Hamilton & Sherman, 1994).

Limitations

Despite its various applications, prototype psychology has some limitations:

Incomplete Representations: Prototypes may not fully capture the complexity of certain categories, leading to oversimplification or misinterpretation of new information (Smith & Medin, 1981).

Oversimplification of Categorization: Prototype theory focuses primarily on similarity-based categorization, which may not fully account for the diverse ways in which people form categories, such as rule-based or goal-driven categorization (Murphy, 2002).

Overreliance on Prototypes: Relying too heavily on prototypes can result in cognitive biases, such as stereotyping, prejudice, and confirmation bias, which can lead to harmful consequences (Hamilton & Sherman, 1994).

FAQs

What is the difference between concept and prototype psychology?

The typically abstract and generalized mental representations of objects, ideas, or experiences are the focus of concept psychology. Concepts are the building blocks of the human mind and are essential for comprehension, communication, and decision-making. (Murphy, 2002). On the other hand, prototype psychology, a particular approach within concept psychology, emphasizes the significance of prototypes, or the most typical and representative examples of a category, in the organization and processing of information. (Rosch, 1973).

Is prototype a category or grouping?

A prototype is not a category or grouping itself but rather a mental representation of the most typical or representative example within a category (Rosch, 1973). Categories in prototype psychology are variable and fluid, with members' levels of likeness to the prototype fluctuating. (Rosch, 1975). In contrast, traditional categorization theories construct categories using rigid definitions and essential characteristics. (Smith & Medin, 1981).

References

Aha, D. W., Kibler, D., & Albert, M. K. (1991). Instance-based learning algorithms. Machine Learning, 6(1), 37-66.

Barsalou, L. W. (1987). The instability of graded structure: Implications for the nature of concepts. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 101-140). Cambridge University Press.

Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, S. J. (1994). Stereotypes. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of Social Cognition (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 1-68). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Keller, K. L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing customer-based brand equity. Journal of Marketing, 57(1), 1-22.

Medin, D. L., & Ortony, A. (1989). Psychological essentialism. In S. Vosniadou & A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 179-195). Cambridge University Press.

Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85(3), 207-238.

Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. MIT Press.

Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77(3), 353-363.

Rosch, E. (1973). Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 4(3), 328-350.

Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104(3), 192-233.

Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27-48). Lawrence Erlbaum.

Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7(4), 573-605.

Smith, E. E., & Medin, D. L. (1981). Categories and concepts. Harvard University Press.

ID, 'custom_sentence', true); if (!empty($custom_sentence)) { ?>
: ""

Cite this page: N., Sam M.S., "PROTOTYPE in Psychology," in PsychologyDictionary.org, April 28, 2013, https://psychologydictionary.org/prototype/ (accessed December 7, 2023).
Share this Article
Facebook Twitter Email Copy Link Print
Avatar photo
By N., Sam M.S.
Sam holds a masters in Child Psychology and is an avid supporter of Psychology academics.
Leave a comment Leave a comment

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Latest Posts

Abusive Parenting
Breaking the Cycle: Understanding the Psychological Roots of Abusive Parenting
Tantrums in Three-Year-Olds
Meltdowns Unveiled: Decoding the Whys and Hows of Tantrums in 3-Year-Olds
Classical Conditioning
The Art of Association: Delving into the Depths of Classical Conditioning
Unlocking the Secrets of Primary Reinforcers: The Key to Understanding Human Motivation
Strict Parents
The Psychology of Strict Parenting Explored
Parental Poison: The Psychology Behind Toxic Parenting Behaviors
Types of Parenting Styles
Parenting Panorama: Exploring Diverse Parenting Styles
Why Montessori is Bad
Montessori Reconsidered: Exploring Its Underlying Challenges
Negative Punishment Examples
The Painful Truth About Negative Punishment: Examples and Effects
Helicopter Moms
The Hovering Effect: The Psychology Behind Helicopter Moms
Psychology of Horror Films
Psychology of Horror Films
Psychological of Human Sexuality
Psychological of Human Sexuality

Popular Psychology Terms

JUDGMENT
MEDICAL MODEL
HYPERMNESIA
AFFIRMATION
BRAINWASHING
BACKUP REINFORCER
QUALITY
WELL-BEING
AFFILIATIVE BEHAVIOR
MESSAGE-LEARNING APPROACH
PRESSURE
PERSONAL ADJUSTMENT
EXCITATION-TRANSFER THEORY
BEHAVIORAL SEQUENCE

Read This Next

PERSONALITY PATTERN DISTURBANCE

By N., Sam M.S.

PERSONALITY TRAIT THEORY

By N., Sam M.S.

PHANTOM REACTION

By N., Sam M.S.

PHYSIOGNOMIC PERCEPTION

By N., Sam M.S.

PHYSIOGNOMY

By N., Sam M.S.

PICA

By N., Sam M.S.

PICK’S DISEASE

By N., Sam M.S.

PONS

By N., Sam M.S.

About Us

Powered by Psychology Dictionary: the only Free Online Psychology Dictionary

Follow Us

©2023 PsychologyDictionary.org

  • About
  • Contact
  • Advertise
  • Terms of Service
  • Privacy Policy